Methods Toward a Science of Behavior and Experience 10th Edition by William J. Ray – Test Bank
To Purchase this Complete Test Bank with Answers Click the link Below
If face any problem or
Further information contact us At tbzuiqe@gmail.com
Sample Test
Chapter 3 – Developing the Hypothesis
Chapter Outline
Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
Making Our Hypotheses Logical
The “I Wonder What Will Happen if” Hypothesis and Inductive
Reasoning
The “I Expect This Would Happen if” Hypothesis and Deductive
Reasoning
Strong Inference
Creating Testable Research Hypotheses and the Problem of
Measurement
Overview
Ideas Come From Everywhere
Intuition and Revelation
The Scientist’s Guide to Having an Idea
Tools for Library Research
Journals & Books
Computer Databases
The Internet
Conclusion
Chapter Overview
Science attempts to determine the extent to which our ideas
about the world are accurate. To this end, operational definitions guide
the scientist in testing hypotheses, though there may be multiple definitions
of the construct under study. There are two types of hypotheses and each
has a different underlying logic. Strong inference is an important
procedure for examining hypotheses. In addition, testing hypotheses
requires reliable and valid measurement.
An experiment’s origin is often the scientist’s idea that is
translated into a testable hypothesis and includes identification of
independent and dependent variables and their operational definitions.
Ideas for experiments come from a variety of sources as described in the
research accounts of Jude Cassidy (infant-child attachment), Lance Shotland
(bystander behavior), Nora Newcombe (gender differences), and Jeffrey Parker
(children’s friendship). Wallas’ four-stage model of creative
problem-solving illustrates different aspects of the process of
idea-generation.
There are many tools for library research and include books,
journals, and computer databases (e.g., PsycINFO, PubMed, MEDLINE, SCI).
Information from professional organizations, governmental agencies, colleges
and universities can be accessed via the internet.
Chapter Objectives
1. Why
do scientists consider their topics from ever changing perspectives?
2. Testing
our hypothesis about reality requires operational definitions. How does Chapter
3 define operational definitions and how are they applied when bonding our
hypothesis to physical reality?
3. How
is construct validity important to our research procedures and measurements?
4. Logically,
what are the two fundamentally different types of hypotheses and what types of
reasoning do they represent?
5. Specify
the steps involved in inductive and deductive reasoning. Give examples of their
use. What are the limitations of these logical processes?
6. Our
goal in research is to use measurements that are both valid and reliable.
Discuss the relationship between reliability and validity. How would you
select methods of measurement that meet this goal?
7. Describe
the steps of the research process in terms of the decisions you are required to
make at each step.
8. Discuss
several ways to find ideas for research. Relate this to your own system of searching
for ideas.
9. Wallas
(1920) described the scientific process in four stages. Explain each in detail
and formulate examples.
10.
What is the importance of library research in the scientific
process?
11.
What tools are available in helping you to locate scientific
information? Describe their different functions and the benefits of these
methods of information gathering.
12.
Explain the concept of strong inference. Describe the four steps
outlined by Platt (1964) and how this idea relates to psychology.
Teaching Points
Creating operational definitions will challenge many
students. The difficulty is likely due to how “slippery” some concepts
are and that many constructs like anxiety and depression can be tested only
indirectly. Another area of difficulty that students will encounter is
taking general hypotheses and translating them to more specific testable
hypotheses. Consider giving students practice in constructing operational
definitions and testable hypotheses regarding love, fear, motivation, effort,
and concentration. Students should note that several alternative
definitions for each construct exist.
Since inductive and deductive reasoning can be difficult for
some students, emphasize how they underlie the types of hypotheses.
Specifically, emphasizing Figure 3.2 might be a highly effective way to help
students grasp the logic.
Your students might be interested in knowing that there are
actually several types of validity (e.g., convergent, discriminant, predictive,
concurrent, face). These validity types are especially important in the
area of psychometrics.
Ask your students to comment on the reliability and validity of
the tests that they take in their coursework. Challenge them to offer
support of their views.
Share your own experiences with how you developed hypotheses.
Talk about colleagues you know and how they came up with their hypotheses.
It might be instructive if you brought to class several journals
for students to examine. Students would see the types of research
conducted, the format in which articles are written, and the breadth of
research.
Students often enjoy hearing about their professor’s own
research and experiences. Depending on your specific case, you may wish
to describe your experiences with using bound abstracts. How has
computerized databases improved your own research?
Teaching Activities
What
Interests You?
To introduce this chapter, ask students to create a list of
questions regarding behavior and mental processes. You can augment this
activity by having students use online references such as PsycINFO and PubMed
in their areas of interest. Additionally, students could construct an
experiment to address their question. Have students create a general
hypothesis, identify the variables, operationally define them, and translate
the general hypotheses into a testable hypothesis.
Research
Tools
Using Handout 3-1, students are to collect citations from the
tools listed that relate to their area of interest. Ask them to report
back to the class their findings.
Operational
Definitions in TV Commercials
To give practice to students and to hone their critical viewing
skills, record some TV commercials and present them to your students.
Their task is to operationally define some of the more common phrases used in
commercials. For examples, persuasive phrases and buzzwords are good
examples of constructs for students to operationally define:
- tastes
good
- best
value
- quality
service
- consumers’
favorite
- most
watched
- new
and improved
- most
durable
- fastest
- world’s
leading
- world
famous
Analyzing Hypotheses
Ask each student to first formulate a hypothesis. Students can
then analyze their hypotheses, or those formulated by other students, by
identifying the independent variable(s) and dependent variables(s). They should
also indicate if the hypothesis is directional and whether or not the variables
have been adequately operationally defined.
Computer Databases
Consider showing students an actual search on a database. Possibilities
include finding citations of your own research or that of a colleague
using Social Science
Citation Index.
Evaluating Internet Sources
Without doubt, the Internet has changed how information is
disseminated both in the popular culture and in science. But accompanying
the change are concerns for uncritical acceptance of Internet content.
There are a number of web sites that provide guidelines and exercises regarding
the evaluation of internet sources:
http://www.montgomerycollege.edu/Departments/writegt/htmlhandouts/Ten%20C%20internet%20sources.htm
http://muse.widener.edu/~tltr/How_to_Evaluate_9.htm
http://www.fno.org/jun97/eval.html
Wadsworth’s Research Methods Workshop
http://www.wadsworth.com/psychology_d/templates/student_resources/workshops/resch_wrk.html
A section of the Wadsworth’s Research Methods Workshop site
discusses Getting
Ideas for a Study. The section discusses the importance of
observation and talking to experts, as well as the utility of attending various
types of research conventions (plus a list of some major conferences). This is
followed by a review of how to search, and gather ideas from, the literature.
As with each segment of the workshop site, this segment concludes with a brief
quiz (consisting of true/false statements and multiple-choice questions).
Internet Resources
What is Science? (http://www.thebee.se/SCIENCE/Science.htm)
At this website, you’ll find a brief description of the history
and “theory” of science detailing the contributions of the Greeks and Kuhn.
Science Quotes (www.lhup.edu/~dsimanek/sciquote.htm)
This website presents some thought- provoking quotes from noted
scientists and theorists that reveal their perspectives on the definition,
pleasure, and beauty of science.
Pseudoscience
(physics.syr.edu/courses/modules/PSEUDO/pseudo_main.html)
Links to sites on alternative medicine, pseudomedicine,
quackery, therapeutic touch, facilitated communication, and skepticism offer a
rich source of various pseudosciences.
Writing Hypotheses
(http://www.accessexcellence.org/LC/TL/filson/writhypo.html)
The purpose of this lesson is to teach students how and when to
write hypotheses.
Cronbach and Meehl
(psychclassics.yorku.ca/Cronbach/construct.htm)
You can read Cronbach and Meehl’s classic paper on construct
validity at this site.
Construct
Validity (www.socialresearchmethods.net/kb/constval.htm)
You will find more information regarding the different types of
validity (e.g., face, concurrent, criterion-referenced).
Suggested Readings
Douglas, N.E., & Baum, N. (1984). Library research guide to
psychology: Illustrated research strategy and sources. Ann
Arbor; MI: Pierian Press.
Weaver, D. B. (1982). How to do a literature search in psychology.
Dallas, Texas: Resource Press.
Great reference
guides for the beginning researcher.
Research Activities for Students
The goal of the research activities are to: (1) relate
Chapter 3 on an applied learning dimension, and (2) get you involved in
research.
1. Mapping
Out Research Ideas. Give yourself permission to freely express any ideas that
come to mind without any restrictions. Place your pen on the paper and do not
lift it until the instructor notifies you to do so after 5 minutes. Begin
in the middle of a blank piece of paper with the first thought that comes to
mind. Continue by mapping all ideas that come to mind. Your goal is
to use your creativity and fill every bit of white space that you can on your
paper. After your instructor calls time, go back and group your ideas
into general areas. Finally, rank your ideas in order of interest.
Turn this paper in to your instructor and arrange for individual time to
discuss possible research ideas.
2. Daily
Journal of Research Ideas. Keep a daily journal of research
ideas or questions that come to mind. Your goal is one idea per day. This
exercise should become a habit for those who wish to become serious
researchers. You will be amazed at the timing of some of your most
inspirational ideas. Format your ideas into research questions and
hypotheses. Don’t hesitate to use the library in search of ideas and
supporting information. For each idea check the library to determine
whether information in this area is: (a) abundant, (b) scattered, but
with a lot of related/supporting information, or (c) scarce. Turn this in
to your instructor.
3. Gaining
Library Skills. Use online references such as PsycINFO and PubMed to
research articles related to psychology.
Assignment: Conduct
a literature
search on one of the following topics:
* Effectiveness of mentoring at-risk youth
* Utilization of mental health services among Latinos
* Benefits of volunteerism
* Improving pro-environmental behaviors
1. Find
the key words. Use the Thesaurus of Psychological terms or the
online references such as PsycINFO and PubMed to identify
the key words to use in your search.
1. Refine
your search. Combine your key words with AND, OR, NOT, and
report the number of records displayed for each search. Print out this
exercise and turn it in to your instructor.
2. Identify
information in the abstracts. Select and read two
abstracts. Record the following information: Author, title of the
article, journal (year, volume and pages), hypotheses, and a brief summary of
the results.
4. Depression
Among College Students. Developing Hypotheses. Go
to the library and use one of the many resources available (scientific journal
articles, PsycInfo, PubMed,
Psychological Abstracts, etc.) to locate four articles related to the topic of
depression. Determine which types of measurement instruments were used to
measure the construct of depression. Describe how the concept of
depression is operationally defined in each study and how the multitude of
definitions may confuse/clarify and refine the construct of depression.
Finally, after conducting your review of the literature, formulate your own
operational definition of depression as well as a testable research question
related to student depression.
5. Empirical
Test of Clichés. Describe in detail how you might empirically test these
common sense sayings about behavior: Develop research questions for each and
consider possible research designs.
1.
birds of a feather flock together
2. absence
makes the heart grow fonder
3.
it takes one to know one
6. Finding
Psychological Articles and Tests on the Internet.
Search the Internet for information on psychological articles related to
personality and social psychology. Also, search the internet for personality
tests like the Myers-Briggs. Print one psychological article and one
psychological test from your Internet search.
7. Biographies
of Scientists.
Use online references such as PsycINFO and PubMed to find a
biography on a noted scientist in psychology (e.g., Wilhelm Wundt, B. F.
Skinner, Sigmund Freud, Kurt Lewin, Leon Festinger, Fritz Heider, etc.).
How does their life story interface with their contribution to
psychology?
Handout 3-1
Research Project
Research Question:
Tool |
Book title: |
Journal: |
Psychological Abstracts: |
Sources from PsycINFO: |
Internet Source: |
Other: |
TEST BANK
MULTIPLE CHOICE
1. The
chapter advises that researchers, in order to gain new perspectives, should:
a. |
practice formulating testable
hypotheses. |
b. |
ask subjects about their experiences in
research. |
c. |
create thoughtful operational definitions. |
d. |
play with their thoughts. |
ANS:
D
PTS:
1
REF: Introduction
2. How
do scientists accurately communicate their private ideas about the world?
a. |
use operational definitions |
b. |
ask their colleagues for assistance in
writing research articles |
c. |
engage in Wallas’ four-stage model of
creative problem-solving |
d. |
use inductive and deductive reasoning |
ANS:
A
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
MSC: WWW
3. Operational
definitions:
a. |
define concepts in terms of clearly
observable, concrete operations that anyone can repeat. |
b. |
define terms conceptually. |
c. |
define events in terms of constructs. |
d. |
all of these |
ANS: A
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
4. An
advantage of using operational definitions is that:
a. |
they allow sampling to be done
randomly. |
b. |
they allow what Wallas described as
inductive reasoning to occur. |
c. |
these definitions associate hypotheses
to objects and operations in the physical world. |
d. |
they allow confounding variables to
exist. |
ANS:
C
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
5. Professor
Cole is discussing aggression as a dependent variable in his experiment. He
says that aggression is the amount of time that a child holds a toy weapon.
This best illustrates a(n):
a. |
operational definition of playing with
a toy weapon. |
b. |
confounding variable. |
c. |
operational definition of aggression. |
d. |
nominal level of measurement. |
ANS:
C
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
6. For
any construct, there is (are):
a. |
only one operational definition. |
b. |
many possible operational definitions. |
c. |
one operational definition that is
valid and one that is reliable. |
d. |
only one true operational definition. |
ANS:
B
PTS: 1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
7. Which
of the following could not be an operational definition of
intelligence?
a. |
Score on an IQ test |
b. |
Number of rare words read per minute |
c. |
GPA |
d. |
How smart you are on several tests |
ANS:
D
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
8. Which
of these is not an operational definition of anxiety?
a. |
Heart rate |
b. |
Degree of nervousness on a math test |
c. |
Participants’ ratings of their own anxiety
on a 1 to 5 scale |
d. |
Scientist’s rating of participants’
anxiety on a 1 to 5 scale |
ANS:
B
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
MSC: WWW
9. Why
would a researcher want to use multiple measures of depression?
a. |
It would increase the chance that the
research would get financial support. |
b. |
Using multiple measures would increase
test-retest reliability. |
c. |
Because there are several operational
definitions of depression. |
d. |
It makes sense because she is studying
people from different backgrounds. |
ANS:
C
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
10.
What is wrong with the following operational definition of
concentration: time taken to run a mile?
a. |
It is not reliable. |
b. |
It cannot be replicated. |
c. |
It lacks construct validity. |
d. |
It is too precise. |
ANS:
C
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
11.
To make a hypothesis testable, you must:
a. |
operationally define your variables. |
b. |
formulate a broad general question. |
c. |
rely on inductive reasoning. |
d. |
use valid constructs. |
ANS:
A
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
12.
If an operational definition has construct validity:
a. |
it is consistent with the hypotheses. |
b. |
it adequately represents a concept. |
c. |
it relies on inductive reasoning. |
d. |
it will be uncorrelated with other
variables. |
ANS: B
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
13.
Questioning whether the time spent on a manipulation task is a
valid measure of conscientiousness refers to the problem of ____ validity.
a. |
predictive |
b. |
construct |
c. |
internal |
d. |
external |
ANS:
B
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
14.
Hypotheses of the form “I expect this would happen if” are
formulated when:
a. |
we know a lot about a phenomenon. |
b. |
we know little about a phenomenon. |
c. |
we reason inductively. |
d. |
we know a lot about a phenomenon and we
reason inductively. |
ANS:
A
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Logical
MSC: WWW
15.
Imagine that a child drops a ball from her highchair and sees it
fall to the ground. Later, as she sits in her bed, she holds a toy and she
expects that, if she drops the toy outside of her bed, that it will fall to the
floor. In fact, she believes that this type of effect will occur whenever she
drops an object. This best illustrates:
a. |
deductive reasoning. |
b. |
inductive reasoning. |
c. |
theory-driven research. |
d. |
hypothetical reasoning. |
ANS:
A
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Logical
16.
Hypotheses of the form “I wonder what will happen if” are
formulated when:
a. |
we know a lot about a phenomenon. |
b. |
we know little about a phenomenon. |
c. |
we reason inductively. |
d. |
we know a lot about a phenomenon and we
reason inductively. |
ANS:
B
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Logical
17.
Hypotheses of the form “I expect this would happen if” are
associated with:
a. |
deductive reasoning. |
b. |
inductive reasoning. |
c. |
theory-driven research. |
d. |
deductive reasoning and theory-driven
research. |
ANS:
A
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Logical
18.
A scientist has outlined a theory of attention. She formulates a
hypothesis and designs an experiment to test the theory. This type of
experiment is associated with:
a. |
deductive reasoning. |
b. |
inductive reasoning. |
c. |
theory-driven research. |
d. |
deductive reasoning and theory-driven
research. |
ANS: D
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Logical
MSC: WWW
19.
Hypotheses of the form “I wonder what will happen if” are
associated with:
a. |
deductive reasoning. |
b. |
inductive reasoning. |
c. |
theory-driven research. |
d. |
deductive reasoning and theory-driven
research. |
ANS:
B
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Logical
20.
One predicts observations from a well-established theory through
a process of:
a. |
induction. |
b. |
incubation. |
c. |
deduction. |
d. |
validation. |
ANS:
C
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Logical
21.
Which of the following is not a characteristic of a testable
research hypothesis?
a. |
It is stated in terms of operational
definitions. |
b. |
It states how the independent variable
is manipulated. |
c. |
It specifies the relation between the
independent and dependent variables. |
d. |
It reflects both inductive and
deductive reasoning. |
ANS: D
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Logical
22.
The research approach that tests two competing theories in order
to falsify one of them is referred to as:
a. |
induction. |
b. |
illumination. |
c. |
strong inference. |
d. |
verification. |
ANS:
C
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Logical
23.
If a measure is reliable, it:
a. |
accurately measures what it is supposed
to measure. |
b. |
is consistent. |
c. |
is valid. |
d. |
is both consistent and valid. |
ANS:
B
PTS: 1
REF: Creating Testable Research Hypotheses and the Problem
of Measurement
MSC: WWW
24.
A psychologist thinks that he has come up with a new and
improved intelligence test. He gives the test to some subjects several times
and finds that there is a lot of fluctuation in how well a person scores. The
fluctuation seems indicate that the new test lacks:
a. |
measurement. |
b. |
validity. |
c. |
integrity. |
d. |
precision. |
ANS: B
PTS: 1
REF: Creating Testable Research Hypotheses and the Problem
of Measurement
25.
If a measure is accurate, it is said to be:
a. |
consistent. |
b. |
valid. |
c. |
precise. |
d. |
reliable. |
ANS:
B
PTS: 1
REF: Creating Testable Research Hypotheses and the Problem
of Measurement
26.
A measure can be:
a. |
accurate but not valid. |
b. |
reliable but not valid. |
c. |
consistent but not reliable. |
d. |
valid but not accurate. |
ANS:
B
PTS: 1
REF: Creating Testable Research Hypotheses and the Problem
of Measurement
27.
The clock on the classroom wall is exactly one hour off. You
would conclude that the clock is:
a. |
not reliable and not valid. |
b. |
reliable but not valid. |
c. |
not reliable but valid. |
d. |
reliable and valid. |
ANS:
B
PTS: 1
REF: Creating Testable Research Hypotheses and the Problem
of Measurement
28.
As the a researcher sets up an hypothesis, it often takes the
form of a(n):
a. |
quote. |
b. |
greater than less than statement. |
c. |
if then statement. |
d. |
theory. |
ANS:
C
PTS: 1
REF: Creating Testable Research Hypotheses and the Problem
of Measurement
29.
Professor James uses a system that requires a subject to press a
button as soon as they see a light blink. He is measuring their reaction time.
Unfortunately, his equipment is not working properly and every reaction time
recorded randomly fluctuates by plus or minus 10%. You would conclude that the
readings obtained with the equipment are:
a. |
not reliable and not valid. |
b. |
reliable but not valid. |
c. |
not reliable but valid. |
d. |
reliable and valid. |
ANS:
A
PTS: 1
REF: Creating Testable Research Hypotheses and the Problem
of Measurement
MSC: WWW
30.
Which of the following is the best formulated hypothesis?
a. |
A psychologist wonders if caffeine
affects attention. |
b. |
Does caffeine affect attention? |
c. |
If caffeine is ingested, attention will
be enhanced. |
d. |
Caffeine and attention: a relationship? |
ANS: C
PTS: 1
REF: Creating Testable Research Hypotheses and the Problem
of Measurement
31.
What do the stories of Jude Cassidy, Lance Shotland, Nora
Newcombe, and Jeffrey Parker say about science?
a. |
Construct validity is difficult to
achieve. |
b. |
Wallas’ stage of scientific process
does not always conform to reality. |
c. |
It is difficult to formulate testable
hypotheses. |
d. |
Ideas for research can come from a
variety of sources. |
ANS:
D
PTS: 1
REF: Ideas Come From Everywhere
32.
You hear one of your classmates say, “Scientists can’t use their
own experiences in the scientific method. They have to be objective!” What is
your reasoned reaction to this statement?
a. |
The classmate is wrong since scientists
often use their own experiences to create hypotheses. |
b. |
The classmate is wrong since science
never attempts to be objective. |
c. |
The classmate is correct since personal
experiences are always subjective. |
d. |
The classmate is right. One’s own
experiences are always seen from a particular point of view. |
ANS:
A
PTS:
1
REF: Ideas Come From Everywhere
33.
Scientists get their research ideas from:
a. |
reading the work of others. |
b. |
talking about their ideas with
colleagues. |
c. |
prior experience. |
d. |
all of these |
ANS:
D
PTS:
1
REF: Ideas Come From Everywhere
MSC: WWW
34.
The famous scientist, ____, often experienced ideas that just
popped into his consciousness.
a. |
Albert Einstein |
b. |
Graham Wallas |
c. |
Karl Popper |
d. |
Thomas Kuhn |
ANS:
A
PTS:
1
REF: Intuition and Revelation
35.
Wallas found that one of the things that scientists do to solve
problems is to learn all they can about a problem and to examine it from varied
perspectives. This stage of scientific problem-solving is called:
a. |
preparation. |
b. |
incubation. |
c. |
illumination. |
d. |
verification. |
ANS: A
PTS:
1
REF: The Scientist’s Guide to Having an Idea
36.
You’ve recently become interested in dreaming and have read many
articles about it. In which of the following stages are you most likely
to be in?
a. |
illumination |
b. |
induction |
c. |
incubation |
d. |
preparation |
ANS:
D
PTS:
1
REF: The Scientist’s Guide to Having an Idea
37.
Which of the following is not one of Wallas’
four stages of scientific problem solving?
a. |
illumination |
b. |
induction |
c. |
incubation |
d. |
verification |
ANS:
B
PTS:
1
REF: The Scientist’s Guide to Having an Idea
38.
Which of the following statements is false according to Wallas’ four-stage
theory of scientific problem-solving?
a. |
Ideas may pop into your mind even
though you have never thought about the problem before. |
b. |
Ideas may come to you suddenly as if
from nowhere. |
c. |
Insights are usually preceded by
extensive study of a topic. |
d. |
Ideas may not take years to develop. |
ANS:
A
PTS:
1
REF: The Scientist’s Guide to Having an Idea
MSC: WWW
39.
According to Wallas, the solution to a problem begins to come
into consciousness during:
a. |
preparation. |
b. |
illumination. |
c. |
incubation. |
d. |
verification. |
ANS:
B
PTS:
1
REF: The Scientist’s Guide to Having an Idea
40.
The stage in Wallas’ four-stage model that is associated with testing
of the hypothesis is called:
a. |
incubation. |
b. |
induction. |
c. |
verification. |
d. |
revelation. |
ANS:
C
PTS:
1
REF: The Scientist’s Guide to Having an Idea
41.
Professor Barrios is currently testing a hypothesis regarding
psychotherapy. He is in Wallas’ stage of:
a. |
verification. |
b. |
incubation. |
c. |
induction. |
d. |
validation. |
ANS:
A
PTS:
1
REF: The Scientist’s Guide to Having an Idea
MSC: WWW
42.
If you wanted to find relevant research of the past five years
on the effects of nicotine on motor performance, which of the following would
be the best source?
a. |
Index Medicus |
b. |
Journal of Developmental
Psychology |
c. |
PsycINFO |
d. |
the website of Harvard’s Department of
Psychology |
ANS:
C
PTS:
1
REF: Tools for Library Research
43.
When searching in PsycINFO, it is best to use search terms that
are:
a. |
general |
b. |
popular |
c. |
specific |
d. |
none of these |
ANS:
C
PTS:
1
REF: Tools for Library Research
TOP: Tools for Library Research
44.
Information about psychological research may be found in:
a. |
Psychological Abstracts. |
b. |
World Wide Web. |
c. |
PsycINFO |
d. |
all of these |
ANS:
D
PTS:
1
REF: Tools for Library Research
TOP: Tools for Library Research
45.
A researcher is interested in flashbulb memory research. If
using PsycINFO, it might be useful to use the following search term:
a. |
memory. |
b. |
flashbulb. |
c. |
types of memory. |
d. |
flashbulb memory. |
ANS:
D
PTS:
1
REF: Tools for Library Research
MSC: WWW
SHORT ANSWER
1. How
and why does the scientist convert private ideas into specific behaviors?
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
2. Describe
the rationale of having multiple measures of the dependent variable.
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS: 1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
3. Discuss
the relation between operational definitions and construct validity.
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
4. Discuss
the relation between different kinds of hypotheses and inductive and deductive
reasoning.
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Logical
5. Discuss
why research described as “theory driven” is based on deductive reasoning.
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Logical
6. Describe
the importance of reliability and validity in measurement.
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS: 1
REF: Creating Testable Research Hypotheses and the Problem
of Measurement
7. Outline
the steps that should be taken to translate a general research question into a
testable research hypothesis.
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS:
1
REF: Overview
8. Operationally
define the independent and dependent variables in the following hypothesis and
restate it in testable form: Practice makes perfect.
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS:
1
REF: Making Our Hypotheses Concrete
9. What
do the stories of the researcher described in the chapter tell us about the
source of ideas in science?
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS:
1
REF: Ideas Come From Everywhere
10.
Describe the four stages of Wallas’ model of scientific
problem-solving.
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS:
1
REF: The Scientist’s Guide to Having an Idea
11.
Describe the tools that a scientist has to conduct library
research.
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS:
1
REF: Tools for Library Research
12.
How have electronic resources, such as databases and the
Internet, changed how scientists conduct research?
ANS:
Answer not provided.
PTS:
1
REF: Tools for Library Research
Chapter 5 – Description of Behavior Through Numerical
Representation
Chapter Outline
Measurement
Scales of Measurement
Nominal Measurement
Ordinal Measurement
Interval Measurement
Ratio Measurement
Identifying Scales of Measurement
Measurement and Statistics
Pictorial Description of Frequency Information
Descriptive Statistics
Measures of Central Tendency
Measures of Variability
Pictorial Presentations of Numerical Data
Transforming Data
Standard Scores
Measure of Association
Chapter Overview
Behavior is described through measurement that exists at four
basic levels or scales: nominal, ordinal, interval, and ratio.
Statistics are used to help us make sense of data. A frequency
distribution provides a pictorial representation of frequency information from
an experiment. Distributions of scores may take several shapes,
such as a bimodal distribution or skewed distribution. Mean, median, and
mode are commonly used measures of central tendency. The choice of which
one to use depends on the distribution. Variability can be measured by
range and standard deviation. Numerical data can be represented in line
and bar graphs.
Data can be transformed from one scale to another and to meet
statistical assumptions. Standard scores such as z scores allow
comparison of scores to other scores. Association between variables can
be depicted in a scatter diagram and with correlation coefficients, which may
be positive or negative. Correlation does not show causal relationships
between variables, but does have value in prediction.
Chapter Objectives
1. Measurement
theory requires us to ask what two questions when doing research?
2. Compare
and contrast the different scales of measurement.
3. Cite
two ways to display data.
4. What
three measures of central tendency are discussed in the text and what are their
characteristics?
5. Discuss
the concept of variability. Give examples of three different measures of
variability and describe their functions.
6. Why
is it common to transform data? Relate transformation of data to the concept of
z-scores.
7. What
is correlation? How does it aid us in understanding the relationship between
variables?
8. How
would you define a positive correlation? How would you define a negative
correlation?
9. What
can you say about the effect of one variable on the other in correlational
studies?
10.
What information is derived from squaring the correlation
coefficient? How does this relate to variability accounted for?
11.
Why is it important to assess underlying distribution before
calculating correlations?
Teaching Points
There are several helpful points to make regarding scales of
measurement. First, each subsequent scale builds upon the previous
scale. Second, to make reasonable conclusions, one must be sensitive to
the use of statistics for a particular scale. Third, use the acronym
NOIR, the French word for black, as a cue for the scales of measurement.
The chapter makes the excellent point that “statistics do not
know and do not care where your numbers come from.” For instance, you
could calculate a mean from nominal level data, but the result would be
meaningless. Students might recognize this as an example of “garbage in
garbage out.”
Students often do not possess good graphing skills and have
difficulty remembering what variables go on which axes. It would be
helpful to give them practice at graphing by using one or more of the
activities described below.
Remind students that a “0” in a dataset counts in determining
the number of scores in calculating the mean.
Many students think too much about the mean (and central
tendency in general) and not enough about variability. It is useful to
demonstrate both mathematically and graphically that two or more data sets can
have identical means but be very different in terms of how dispersed the values
are in each set.
Another area of challenge for students is how and why skewed
distributions affect the choice of a measure of central tendency, in addition
to difficulty with conceptually understanding standardized scores like
z-scores.
Students may also get hung up on the words positive and negative.
Emphasize that positive correlation does not refer to a good correlation nor
does negative correlation refer to a bad correlation.
Teaching Activities
Using
Students as Subjects
This chapter lends itself well to collecting and analyzing and
creating distributions of data gotten from your students. Here are some
ideas:
- construct
frequency distributions for height, weight, shoe size, and gender
- calculate
measures of central tendency and standard deviation for height and shoe
size
- transform
the scores of height in z-scores
- construct
a scatter diagram with height and shoe size
- calculate
correlation coefficient between height and shoe size
- calculate
correlation coefficient between shoe size and number of siblings
Dissect a Graph
Supply students with a set of graphs and ask them to label and
critique every part. It may be best to use a variety of graphs and to
even include errors. Include graphs with error bars because students
often have trouble interpreting what these indicators of variability mean.
Tables
and Graphs in the Popular Media
Locate tables and graphs used in the popular media and bring
them into class. Let students examine them and formulate
conclusions. Next compare the conclusions generated by the students to
the conclusions that accompanied the table or graph. Augment this
activity with examples of poorly constructed tables and graphs that are
available from the web site described below.
Measures
of Central Tendency in the Popular Media
As the textbook suggests, we hear about these measures often in
the popular media. Bring in examples of articles and ads that refer to a
measure. Suggest that without other information, it is difficult to know
if the appropriate measure was used.
Wadsworth’s Statistics Workshop
http://www.wadsworth.com/psychology_d/templates/student_resources/workshops/stats_wrk.html
The Wadsworth’s Statistics Workshop site contains links to
several sections relevant to students after reading the chapter. For
example, Scale of
Measurement discusses the four levels of measurement of
variables in psychological research, their abstract number properties, and the
types of statistics that are permitted for each variable. The tutorial ends
with a summary and students can test their knowledge in identifying scales of
measurement by doing the practice exercises. Another workshop entitled Central Tendency and Variability provides
information on how to summarize various kinds of data. Students are led through
an example that exercises their skill in calculating measures of central
tendency and variability. The workshop segment z Scores elaborates on the value of
the z score and provides a step-by-step process of calculating z. Finally, the
section on Correlation describes
the types of correlations (positive, negative, and zero) and introduces the
formula for the Pearson’s correlation coefficient.As with each segment of the
workshop site, this segment concludes with a brief quiz.
Internet Resources
Measurement theory
(ftp://ftp.sas.com/pub/neural/measurement.faq)
This site provides additional information on measurement theory,
scales of measurement, and transformations.
The Best and Worst of Statistical
Graphics (www.math.yorku.ca/SCS/Gallery/)
This site has some interesting examples of poorly conceptualized
and created attempts at representing data visually.
More resources on statistics
(http://www.statsoft.com/textbook/)
Elementary statistical concepts are
explained concisely and effectively. Of particular interest is the
statistical glossary.
Datasets (lib.stat.cmu.edu/datasets)
(http://www.umass.edu/statdata/)
If you need real data to illustrate
statistical concepts, these sites have many datasets from a variety of
disciplines. The second site groups the datasets by what statistics would
be appropriate to analyze the data.
Statistics links (www.mccombs.utexas.edu/faculty/jonathan.koehler/links/statistics.asp)
This site contains many links to interesting articles and
activities related to statistics. Some of the topics addressed by the sites
include games, gambling, sports, correlation, and comprehensive statistics
sites.
Suggested Readings
Huff, D., & Geis, I. (1994). How to lie with statistics.
New York: W. W. Norton.
This amusing book is easy
reading full of actual examples of statistical misuses.
Research Activities for Students
The goals of the research activities are to: (1) relate
Chapter 5 on an applied learning dimension, and (2) get you involved in
research.
1. Physical
Activity and Mood: A Correlational Study. For
one week record the information using the following scales:
Rate your level of
physical activity on the following scale:
1. Very
physically active
2. Moderately
physically active
3. Somewhat
physically active
4. Not
very physically active
5. Very
physically inactive
Rate your level of
mood on the following scale:
1. Very
good mood
2. Relatively
good mood
3. Neutral
4. Somewhat
of a bad mood
5. Very
bad mood
Date Time of
day Physical Activity
Rating Mood Rating
1.
2.
3.
4.
5.
6.
7.
Research Flowchart: Fill in the
following information.
1. State
your research hypothesis.
1. Operationally
define your variables.
1. State
measures you controlled or held constant.
1. Graph
data on a scatterplot. Label the axes and the graph.
1. Calculate
by using the Pearson’s correlation coefficient.
1. Interpret.
State the coefficient of determination.
2. Parents,
Sex Information, and Risk Taking Behavior: A Correlational Study. Administer
the following survey to at least 15 individuals.
This survey involves information regarding sexual attitudes and
behavior. I will be using the information gathered from this survey to
gain knowledge about graphing data. No names should be indicated on the
survey, and all information will be kept completely confidential. If
there are any questions that you do not wish to answer, please feel free to skip
to the next question. Thank you for your participation.
What is your gender? Male __________ Female
__________
For the following questions please circle your response:
1, 2, 3, 4 or 5.
1. Did
your parents discuss sex and its risks with you while you lived at home as an
adolescent?
1
2
3
4
5
Not at all
Very
much
2. How knowledgeable
do you believe you are regarding sexually transmitted diseases, AIDS, and the
risks of pregnancy?
1
2
3
4
5
Not at all
Very
much
3. How
much do you feel the information that your parents shared about sex and its
risks has influenced your willingness to engage in safe sexual behaviors (e.g.,
using condoms)?
1
2
3
4
5
No influence
Significant
1. State
your research hypotheses.
2. Organize
the data obtained from questions 2 and 3 into a scatterplot. The abscissa
should be labeled “Degree of Knowledge” (values 1-5). The ordinate should
be labeled “Degree of Influence” (values 1-5).
3. Calculate
a Pearson product moment correlation coefficient between for the data obtained
from questions 1 and 2, questions 1 and 3, and questions 2 and 3.
4. Interpret
the results by explaining what type of relationships, if any, are supported by
the data. Discuss inferences about causation.
3. Misleading
with the Average. Entry level salary data was collected on small sample of
college graduates who earned degrees in psychology and biology. Find the
mean, median, and mode for each group. If you wanted to persuade someone
that they should definitely pursue a degree in psychology, which measure of central
tendency would you use? If you wanted to emphasize that it really didn’t
matter what they majored in, which measure of central tendency would you use?
Entry Level Salaries for Graduates in:
Psychology
Biology
$30,000
$30,000
$36,000
$36,000
$40,000
$40,000
$42,000
$42,000
$149,000
$49,000
4. Misleading
with Statistics. Use online references such as PsycINFO and PubMed to find
articles about how statistics can lie. Explain this effect. Also see the
suggested reading list at the end of this chapter.
5. Assess
Your Self Esteem. Complete the following questions. Identify each item
as either a nominal, ordinal, interval, or ratio number.
Social Security Number _____
Male (01) _____ Female (02) _____
Year of Birth _____
Age _____
Class Standing:
Freshman (1) _____ Sophomore (2) _____ Junior (3) _____ Senior
(4) _____
Exact time you started this test? ______
Comments
Post a Comment